Critique of baby shaking prosecutions raises troubling response

The Age

Greg Barns

Barrister

May 25, 2021

The response of Victorian forensic scientists to a critique of their methods in assessing socalled baby shaking cases shows that there is some disagreement among those in this field.

Perhaps this ought to be the trigger for a much-needed independent inquiry and review of the use of forensic science and evidence in the criminal justice system. The decision of attorneys-general across Australia to abandon such a review, commissioned in 2019, should be reversed immediately.

As The Age has revealed, an article by Dr Chris Brook, published in 2019 in the Australian Journal of Forensic Sciences, took issue with a 2018 conviction of Joby Rowe, who was jailed for nine years for the homicide of his three-month-old daughter. The case involved the evidence of forensic pathologist Dr Linda Iles and forensic paediatrician Dr Jo Tully.

Their evidence was based on a “triad” of internal head injuries – bleeding in the brain, retinal haemorrhage and swelling of the brain – as the cause of death.

As The Age reported, “the diagnosis that ‘triad-only’ injuries are caused by the violent shaking of a baby is now hotly contested globally. Proponents claim the triad is conclusive of abuse, while detractors say faulty science means such convictions are unsound.” Dr Brook is one of those critics . After correspondence,the article was pulled from the Journal website. Whether Drs Tully and Iles on the one hand and Dr Brook on the other are correct, or if both have valid points, is a matter that seems unresolved. But then it is not unusual in science to have disagreements between professionals over such issues as methods and theories.

The concerns about forensic evidence and science in the criminal justice system is, however, not new.

There have been with rumblings of doubt in the medical and legal world, including judicial comment, in recent years.

The response by a senior Victorian prosecutor, Ray Gibson QC, to this controversy over the methodologies and theories in so-called shaken baby cases fails to grapple with the essence of the issue. According to Mr Gibson when an expert witness “gives an opinion, they are subject to cross-examination. It’s open to scrutiny, it’s open to be contradicted by another expert giving a contrary opinion. If we as the prosecution can’t exclude that then the prosecution fails.”

Dr Chris Brook, astrophysicist and critic of shaken baby syndrome.

This reminder breezes over the issue of the president of Victoria’s Court of Appeal, Chris Maxwell, has identified as the “scientific illiteracy” of those who participate in a trial. Justice Maxwell has for some years expressed his concern about “the problem of unreliable science and wrongful convictions” and their effect on the criminal justice system.

In his 2017 paper, Preventing Miscarriages of Justice: the Reliability of Forensic Evidence and the Role of the Trial Judge as Gatekeeper, he notes that the responsibility for ensuing the reliability of evidence.

In July 2019 a ground-breaking and disturbing analysis by a researcher at the University of Sydney led by Dr Jason Chin, a law and psychology lecturer, found after examining 30 forensic science journals that “these journals aren’t requiring authors to conduct their research transparently, such as by posting their data online for others to scrutinise.

These are the closed practices that in other fields contributed to studies showing medicines worked  Forensic science practices and methodologies, like all forms of science, need regular reviewing.

That is not to say Dr Iles and Dr Tully were wrong, and Dr Brook right, it is just that if there is real disagreement about something so critical as forensic evidence in the criminal justice process Victoria’s Attorney-General Jaclyn Symes should go it alone on such a review if her counterparts across the nation refuse to allow the national review they commissioned two years ago to begin its work.

At the very least, surely there should be a pause in current prosecutions in cases where a person is charged in relation to the death of a child and where the state relies on hotly contested methods and theories such as “triad” evidence so that experts like Dr Iles and Dr

Tully and colleagues can take stock of the state of the science.

Greg Barns SC is a barrister and National Criminal Justice Spokesman for the Australian Lawyers Alliance.



Decoding 'the Most Complex Object in the Universe'

June 14, 2013 

Heard on Talk of the Nation

The human brain contains some 100 billion neurons, which together form a network of Internet-like complexity. Christof Koch, chief scientific officer of the Allen Institute for Brain Science, calls the brain "the most complex object in the known universe," and he's mapping its connections in hopes of discovering the origins of consciousness.

Guests:

Christof Koch, Chief Scientific Officer, Allen Institute for Brain Science

Patricia Kuhl, Director, NSF Science of Learning Center, Professor and Co-Director, Institute for Learning and Brain Sciences, University of Washington.

The human brain contains some 100 billion neurons, which together form a network of Internet-like complexity. Christof Koch, chief scientific officer of the Allen Institutefor Brain Science, calls the brain "the most complex object in the known universe," and he's mapping its connections in hopes of discovering the origins of consciousness.

Guests:

Christof Koch, Chief Scientific Officer, Allen Institute for Brain Science

Patricia Kuhl, Director, NSF Science of Learning Center, Professor and Co-Director, Institute for Learning and Brain Sciences, University of Washington

RESEARCH NEWS

President Obama Calls For A BRAIN Initiative

This is SCIENCE FRIDAY. I'm Ira Flatow. Your brain has nearly 100 billion neurons, and one of my next guests compares that complexity to the Amazon rainforest. In fact, he says there about as many trees in the Amazon as there are neurons in your brain.

Think about what the Amazon looks like for a second.

And the roots and the branches and the leaves and the vines, all of that can be compared to the tangled network formed between your brain cells because many of your neurons are in fact wired to tens of thousands of other neurons. That incredible complex network is packed into a soft, three pound organ inside your head, making it, as my next guest says, the most complicated object in the known universe.

Is it possible we'll figure out the secrets of what's inside our heads? I mean, if we are inside that black box, can we understand the whole black box? Like can you see the forest for the trees? Is consciousness beyond our understanding?

My next guests are both decoding the brain's inner workings by observing how we learn and how we speak, by eavesdropping on how individual neurons, the neurons,the cells, speak to one another. Christof Koch is chief scientific officer at the Allen Institute for Brain Science here in Seattle. He's also the author of "Consciousness: Bad Diagnosis For New Psychiatry 'Bible'

FLATOW: Good morning. Patricia Kuhl is director of NSF Science of Learning Center.

She's also a professor and co-director of the Institute for Learning and Brain Sciences

at the University of Washington here in Seattle. Welcome to SCIENCE FRIDAY.

PATRICIA KUHL: Thank you.

FLATOW: Is it possible - let me begin with you, Dr. Koch. Do you think that consciousness might be possible to figure out? I mean, how can we get inside that black box? If we're in the black box, can we see what it's made out of?

KOCH: So for the past 3,000 years that we've been trying to understand the mindbody problem, mainly by philosophy, mainly by talking about it, we've not been very successful. Philosophers are very good at asking questions, but historically they're pretty bad at answering them in any decisive way.

(LAUGHTER)

KOCH: But fortunately, since 150 years, we have clinical science, we have neurology,and we can study what happens if you have a hole in your head and if you have - depending where the damage is, you might have a specific loss of consciousness. So we know, unlike the Greeks, we now know consciousness rises from the brain, and we know if you lack particular parts of the brain, you will lack specific content of 

And over the last 50 years, particularly in animals but also in humans, we've been able to look at actually the stuff out of which the brain is made, those many tangled neurons that Ira was just mentioning.

There's an enormous number of them, and just like as in the rainforest, there's an enormous diversity of them, and that's the complexity we're facing just like in astrophysics over the last 100 years, every new - each new generation of astronomers and astrophysicists discovers that the universe is yet more bigger - yet bigger than we previously thought.

Now people talk about multiverses. Each time we look with better and better tools, with better with better microscopes and other tools, we see more and more complexity. So now we realize there are not just two types of nerve cells, but there are a probably 1,000 different types of nerve cells, just like there are 1,000 different species of trees in the rainforest.

And so the challenge is try to unravel it(ph) . And we have - the atoms of consciousness are really, are the nerve cells. And so really it's essential for us if we want to understand the mind-body problem, in health and in disease, in schizophrenia and other pathologies of consciousness, we really need to understand it at this neuronal level.

FLATOW: And are you actually able to go in to the neuronal level and understandthat? Is it useful to study individual neurons? Could that tell us more about consciousness?

KOCH: It's essential. The only way we're going to find out about, totally uncover the underpinnings of consciousness, how - the brain is a physical system like any other physical system in the universe. But it exudes this magic thing, these feelings. I wake up each morning, I open my eyes, I have pains and pleasure. I remember who I am.

How does that arise out of the physics of the brain?

And the only way we can study that is by actually looking at the stuff that computes this stuff that exudes mind. And the stuff that exudes mind is brain. It's not just some oozy, fluffy, tofu-like substance or overcooked cauliflower. It actually consists out of roughly 100 billion nerve cells, and they interact in a very complex way.

And we know from certain mental diseases, you know, I mentioned schizophrenia, there's also Alzheimer's and Parkinson's, it all involves various, various complicated mis-wiring. And so in order to help people ultimately, we need to understand the wiring and the mis-wiring, and that can primarily be done at the level of individual nerve cells.

FLATOW: Dr. Kuhl, let's talk about how much of your brain, how much of whom you are now, your consciousness, were you born with and how much did you pick over the years, I mean the nature-nurture question, right?

KUHL: Right, so we're interested in taking the approach of studying consciousness and all the magical things that humans can do by studying the baby, starting with baby brains and young children's brains and looking at the explosion of learning that takes place early.

And the tools of modern neural science now allow us to look and see activity. We can take snapshots of the brain and see individual areas, 433 of them, and the white and gray matter is their form. You can look at the fiber tracks that are connecting areas.

You can look at activity using magnetoencephalography.

That's happening online as baby hears a word, sees their mother or smells something that they recognize as milk.

FLATOW: Right. Your brain is made up of all these neurons, and yet we have all this DNA that we're born with.

There's got to be, I imagine, somewhere along the line where the DNA gets triggered to mature or create something that helps your brain mature and develop.

KUHL: Exactly, exactly.

FLATOW: Is that where the nature part comes in on it?

KUHL: So think of the baby as growing up in an ecosystem, and part of the ecosystem is what the baby brings to it.

There's a brain, and that brain's got windows of opportunity that are being opened by genes that are expressing. And also those windows of opportunity narrow. So you've got the baby trajectoring along that developmental path with a set of biological markers that are set to go off at certain times.

Then the ecosystem is the rest of us, the social environment. It's the people, the things, the schools, whether the child's growing up in poverty, that will provide or not provide what that critical window of opportunity is supposed to be receiving.

And so the baby's not on a turnkey system entirely. The baby's waiting for all of us to provide the necessary stuff.

FLATOW: Well, you mention a critical window of opportunity, and we all know how kids learn things like sponges, right? They suck them up much faster than we can. Can we find out how that works and extend that into later life?

KUHL: Well, there's a lot of things that we're doing to try to understand that. So when windows open for learning and close for learning, there are triggers.

We're trying to understand what the triggers are. We're trying to understand whether you can move them so that they open later or close later. And some of the magic that kids are putting to work has to do with how they interact with the social world.

So in learning of language, which we know we're geniuses at between zero and seven but not so good every two years after the age of seven, you're falling off the curve with regard to your ability to learn a new language. After puberty, it really gets difficult.

And so what we're looking at is, well, what's the baby brain doing at, you know, age two or age three that you and I can't do anymore. And what the baby brain's doing is calculating, taking statistics as they listen to us talk and interact in the environment and also reacting to us socially.

The social brain has a good bit to do in controlling how babies react to the statistics of their world.

FLATOW: Could it also be what the baby brain is not doing, which is our taxes, our school, balancing our budget, those sort of - the noise?

(LAUGHTER)

KUHL: Right. Well, the other part of the baby brain is to actually be able to look.

We can now look with diffusion tensor imaging at the superhighways in the brain.

You can see how language areas and executive control areas are related to the reward system.

You know, that squirt of dopamine that I hope you're all getting now as you listen to me talk, and you're imaging your little child and your little grandchild, that squirt of dopamine in the reward system has a lot to do with how it is that we learn at different ages.

So understanding what drives human learning at different ages, and it's different over your - over the lifespan, will help us, help all children because that critical first five years, when typical kids are cruising along, but kids with developmental disabilities are not, is very, very important and also for all of us who want to keep our brains alive all our lives.

Understanding how it works is extremely important.

FLATOW: Dr. Koch, do we have a definition, does everybody agree on a definition of what consciousness is?

KOCH: No, nobody agrees.

(LAUGHTER)

KOCH: There's no universal agreement. But there's widespread - sort of it relates to the feeling, it feels like something. It feels like something to have pain. It feels like something to be interviewed by you. It feels like something to be a man.

Or those are all different...

FLATOW: How about a dog? I mean, a dog must have - feels like something, right?

KOCH: There's no question that dogs, in fact all mammals, have conscious states, right?

There's no question your dog can feel happy, he can feel sad. You know, people do, babies do, patients do. What we don't know how far it goes outside mammals.

Is it possible that - some people are certain, I believe that most, if not all, multicellular. 

A bee is a very complicated system. The brain of a bee is 10 times more heavily wired than we are.

Of course it has many fewer neurons, roughly a million neurons, but it may well feel like something to be a bee. And so the question is: How does this feeling, how does this subjective feeling come into the world? That's always been the mystery. It's always been the central mystery about consciousness.

FLATOW: Now, Eric Kandel spent his whole lifetime looking at cells of a sea slug and won the Nobel Prize eventually for it.

Can you bring that to a higher level? Because he was probing an individual cell almost like you're talking about probing individual cells  learn more about how consciousness is from those individual cells?

KOCH: Yes. So, ultimately, what we're doing - and President Obama is launching a new initiative to try to speed this up - is to record, to listen in. So you can put a piece of wire into a human brain during neurosurgery, or you can do it in animal brains, and you can listen to the way neurons chat to each other. They have this sort of hash...

(SOUNDBITE OF MAKING SOUNDS)

KOCH: It works very well if you're German.

(LAUGHTER)

KOCH: I mean, the brain itself doesn't sound like anything (unintelligible).

FLATOW: We actually have a sound of the brain. We're going to play - we have - give us a minute. We actually have - I think is probably some audio that you...

KOCH: Yes.

FLATOW: ...gave to us. Let's listen to that now.

(SOUNDBITE OF CRACKLING)

FLATOW: Tell us what we're listening to.

KOCH: So here, we're listening to sound track that's made by putting a piece of wire into, in this case, a human brain during neurosurgery and amplifying the electrical signal. And each of those little clicks is actually a little pulse that's being sent by one neuron to roughly - we don't know - anywhere between 5,000 and 10,000 other neurons. And so you have to imagine, as we talk right now, you know, there are literally billions of neurons that chatter away in this code either - and they're sending them out, and they're receiving it. And somehow, out of this cacophony arises, youknow, this stable perception arises the fact that I can - I have a voice in my head, and it sounds like you. And so that's...

(LAUGHTER)

FLATOW: Sorry for that.

(LAUGHTER)

FLATOW: Yeah. And you think that you can actually learn something - was that - just -is it being too simplistic to learn the macro size of something from the individual pulses in the brain?

KOCH: Well, by and large, science has been...

FLATOW: Too reductionist here, I guess is what I'm asking.

KOCH: Well, but science has been very successful over the last 200 years at exactly taking this...

FLATOW: Yeah.

KOCH: ...ever more reductionist point of view. But the good question you ask is that maybe consciousness requires more than just a reductionist approach. And so there are some theories that try to address that, that, in fact, argue that the part of the brain that's not reducible, in some sense, that's what gives rise to consciousness.

So we don't know.

But it has been so successful, so that's, of course, what we should do. In the meantime, we're discovering all sorts of other interesting facts about the brain, but there's really no good alternative to doing it this way.

FLATOW: Dr. Kuhl, you - one area you do a lot of work on is in language. Tell us what you're trying to find out about language.

KUHL: Language is one of the quintessential human abilities, and we're trying to understand how it comes together in the human brain. As Christof says, we're - weknow that the activity is happening at the neuronal level, but it's when millions of neurons are coordinated to do something like listen to a word and recognize it, to have an idea in your head requires this - that you will compute all the statistics and create an idea. We're trying to understand how that happens in the baby brain, and how being bathed in one language, as opposed to another, or being bathed in two at the same time - as in bilingual children - how does that change the whole brain? And, in fact, it does.

What we're seeing is that areas of the brain are different. The connectivity is different.

The superhighways are constructed differently in bilingual and monolingual brains.

And, in fact, bilingual brains are more creative. They're more - they're not smarter. It's not your general IQ. But it is your ability to be a flexible thinker. When solving a problem, if you're bilingual - whether you're a baby or whether you're an adult - you're going to be better at solving a novel problem.

So we have all these new imaging techniques, not looking at individual neurons, but when thousands of neurons fire in coordination and change current flow, we have babies and adults sitting in something that looks like a hairdryer from Mars, but it's completely safe and completely noninvasive and absolutely silent. So we can play things for themselves, show them pictures, show them movies and watch their brains as they are processing real words, and link that to their structures.

We have like a - with MRI and DTI, you have a static photograph of that individual child's brain, the connections in that brain, and how this activity fires in the various areas. So that's brand new and very, very exciting.

 We're talking about brain, listening and learning from the brain. Is there a tool - I'm going to give you the same question I gave to Christopher - to Christof. If I could give you a blank check and you could invent a machine to study the brain, what would it be, and what would you like to see?

KUHL: Well, we want to see the next generation of something like magnetoencephalography. I mean, it is picking up a current flow, and it's able to see with millimeter accuracy, but we want to get even smaller than that - a millisecond accuracy where the activity is in the brain, what's the cortical dynamics that's going on as kids learn. And how does the structure that was built yesterday operate today as the kid looks forward in their world? So we want better and more efficient machines.

We want things that will allow us to see with absolute clarity how one brain differs from another. And why is that important? Well, we just showed in a study published May 29th that if you take a simple brain measure on a child with autism at the age of two, you can predict better than any other measure that we have how that children's - that child's outcomes will look at age four and age six, not only in language.

This is a language probe at the age of two, how the brain responds to known words, but how that child behaves throughout the language arena and cognition and adaptive behavior, and it gets stronger over time.

So these brain measures are not only going to reveal the wonder of the human brain for all of us over the age - over our ages, but for children who have disabilities, we can get in there early with biomarkers to say where in the brain - one, this child's at risk, but, two, where in the brain, what structures, what superhighways are missing? What might we do to create better interventions that don't miss that window of opportunity?

We've got to get in there really early when the brain is plastic and change the course of that child's life and that child's family's life.

FLATOW: Mm-hmm. Let me get a quick question from the audience. We've got about

a minute. Yes, sir.

JORDAN BROWN: OK. My name is Jordan Brown. And I was wondering - you've been talking about processing information from outside, like language and different things, but where does that go? What is the physical thing that constitutes thought? And could you find that by just tracking the electrical signals from the outside inputs?

FLATOW: That's such a good question.

KUHL: Good question.

(LAUGHTER)

KUHL: Would you like a job?

FLATOW: And I'm going - he asked - yes - signed him up, as we used to say in baseball. I'm going to hold that for the break, because we have a break coming up in about 30 seconds. And we'll have - well, that's a meaty question to tackle in 30 seconds.

(LAUGHTER)

FLATOW: And, yeah, we're thinking he wants a job. We'll come back and talk more with Christof Koch, chief scientific officer of the Allen Institute for Brain Science here in Seattle, and Patricia Kuhl, director of the NSF Science and Learning Center, also professor and co-director of the Institute for Learning and Brain Sciences at the University of Washington here in Seattle. We're going to take, come back after this break and step up to the mike with your questions. This is a good time for them. We'll be right back after this break. Stay with us.

(SOUNDBITE OF MUSIC)

FLATOW: I'm Ira Flatow. This is SCIENCE FRIDAY, from NPR.

(SOUNDBITE OF MUSIC)

FLATOW: This is SCIENCE FRIDAY. I'm Ira Flatow.

We're talking this hour about the brain, how a complicated tangle of neurons can give rise to consciousness. Talking with my guests, Christof Koch, chief scientific officer of the Allen Institute for Brain Science here in Seattle, Patricia Kuhl, director of the NSF Science of Learning Center, also professor and co-director of the Institute for Learning and Brain Sciences at the University of Washington here in Seattle.

And before the break, a very good question came in from a young member of our audience, wanting to know, OK, so what happens to all the stuff you - that flows into your brain, all the information and stuff? What happens to it? Christof, you want to tackle that? Yeah?

KOCH: Jordan, so I think it's a very good question. So the brain is, in some sense, similar to a computer. In both cases, you have information that flows in, in one case through transistors, in the other case through nerve cells in our eyes. For example, if I look at you. And then as you said, we have to track - just like in a computer, we can track so that the electric activity in the various transistors, we can do the same in the brain. We can track the electrical activity, either using the methods that Patricia Kuhl alluded to from the outside, using EG or EMG, or we can put microelectrodes in it or other tools, and we can see how the electric activity moves through the brain and activates language area or activates a part of the brain that's responsible for seeing, or another part of the brain that's responsible for hearing. And then ultimately, it activates part of the brain that's responsible for motion, and then we move our eyes, or we speak.

(LAUGHTER)

FLATOW: You talk - you've brought in some references to computer, transistors and things like that. In your book, you compare the brain with its hundred billion neurons to the Internet, which has several billion computers there. Could consciousness arise from the Internet?

(LAUGHTER)

FLATOW: We know what - we know what's out there now. But could something meaningful like consciousness arise from the Internet?

KOCH: In principle, yes. In practice, we have no idea, and I don't think it's a useful research topic right now. But in principle...

(LAUGHTER)

FLATOW: Why not?

KOCH: Well, I mean, let's first look at the analogy. The brain is a very complicated network that consists of individual elements called nerve cells. OK? Now, of course, it doesn't make any sense to say one nerve cell conscious. But a large collection of 

So now I look at another network, very, very complex, the Internet. It has 10 billion nodes.

Each node consists of, you know, between two and 10 billion transistors. So maybe it's also possible that collective, as a whole, it also feels like something to be the Internet. And if you would turn it off, it wouldn't feel anything anymore.

But right now, we don't have a good way to actually test this. Right now, that's purely science fiction.  So you're not going to get a grant funded to study whether the Internet is conscious or not.

FLATOW: But you think it might be, you said, someday.

KOCH: Yeah, in...

FLATOW: In principle.

KOCH: ...in principle (unintelligible) there's no reason. Of course, we see it in, you know, "Matrix" or other movies all the time, that, you know, machines become sentient. In principle, nothing in the universe - there's no physical law that says that that is not possible.

FLATOW: All right. Let me get - time for maybe one more question up here. Yes.

DANTE MANTEL: Yes. Dante Mantel(ph), of Northwest University in Kirkland. You kind of answered most of my question. In science fiction, you just have a big enough computer, and it might become conscious. So - but expanding on that, do you use computer simulations, and is it helpful for studying the brain?

KOCH: Yeah, it's essential. So we - for example, right now, at the Allen Institute, we have this very large project where we're recording from brains, human and animal brains, but we also are simulating it. Because even if you could record from - even if you could visualize the activity of every single nerve cell in something I call a brain TV - imagine a television with something like 200,000 pixel by 200,000 pixel. And every pixel is the activity of one single nerve cell, and sometimes it turns white when it fires one of these pulses, and sometimes it usually goes black.

And now I'm looking at this picture of 100 billion pixel, right? I probably wouldn't understand any of it.

So I need a theory. I need a model. I need a theory to say, OK, this is language and this is vision. And it's vision for the following reason, and it's language for the other reason. And this happens, you know, if you look at the brain TV of a schizophrenic patient because it's hardly different, and that's why the person hears a voice. And so it's essential to theories, and then it's actually essential to the test in using computers.

FLATOW: Mm-hmm. Is it possible to make a computer where the brain picture that big, with all the neurons firing?

KOCH: Not right now, but there's a group, you may have heard. There's a very large project. It was just funded to the tune of 1 billion euro in the European Union called the Human Brain Project, where they're exactly trying to do that. Over the next 10 years, they're to build a special-purpose supercomputer, because you need - you know,

it's terabytes of data. So they're trying to build a special-purpose computer to simulate the human brain.

FLATOW: Wow. It's quite interesting and quite...

KOCH: It's pretty cool stuff.

FLATOW: It's pretty cool stuff. And we want to thank Doctor Kuhl for coming with - to be with us today. Patrick Kuhl - Patricia Kuhl is director of the NSF Science of Learning Center. She's also a professor and co-director of the Institute for Learning and Brain Sciences at the University of Washington in Seattle. And also Christof Koch is chief scientific officer at the Allen Institute for Brain Sciences here in Seattle. Thank you both for taking time to be with us today.

KOCH: Thank you very much.

KUHL: Thank you.



Jockey Lorna Brooke dies after fall at Taunton racecourse early in April

The Guardian

Tony Paley

Tue 20 Apr 2021 

Leeds United Racecourses

Racecourses on Monday observed a minute’s silence and jockeys wore blackarmbands after the announcement that 37-year-old amateur rider LornaBrooke had died in hospital on Sunday following a fall from her mother’s horse at Taunton this month.

Brooke had been riding Orchestrated for owner-trainer Lady Susan Brooke when taking a heavy fall at the third fence in a handicap chase on 8 April.

Brooke’s injuries resulted in racing being delayed by more than hour as she was treated on track, before being transferred into an air ambulance.

The jockey was subsequently treated for a suspected spinal injury, but following complications had been placed in an induced coma from which she never emerged.

The Injured Jockeys Fund, which issued the statement, said: “Her family thank everyone for their kindness in the last few weeks, particularly the staff at Southmead Hospital who were so professional. They will be having a private funeral and will hold a celebration of Lorna’s life once Covid restrictions allow.”

The last jockey to die on a British racecourse as a result of a fall was Tom Halliday at Market Rasen in July 2005. It had been nearly a decade before that that Richard Davis lost his life at Southwell in 1996, while Philip Barnard was killed at Wincanton in 1991 and Vivian Kennedy at Huntingdon in 1988.

Brooke had ridden in more than 400 races, mostly for her mother, who was based in Llandrindod Wells, since she started riding in public and had partnered 17 winners in Britain and Ireland since the 2001-02 season.

She enjoyed her highest-profile success in the inaugural Ladies Handicap Chase at Fairyhouse on Moonlone Lane for Paul Stafford in 2015. A 25-1 chance, Moonlone Lane had failed to win in a 26-race career up to that point, but Brooke guided him home in front, with prominent female riders Katie Walsh, Lizzie Kelly, Rachael Blackmore and Nina Carberry all finishing in her wake.

“It’s tragic news, it really is shocking,” Stafford told Sky Sports Racing.

“When she got the fall people were very concerned, we were too and we were saying prayers, but unfortunately this has happened. She was a lovely person, obviously she rode that winner for us and we kept in contact over the years through social media, congratulating each other on social media when each of us had winners. It’s shocking it’s happened.”

Thinking back to the day at Fairyhouse, Stafford added: “He’d never won a race until she rode him that day, she took the bull by the horns, went on and it paid off – she gave him a fantastic ride. I’d never met Lorna before that, she was more or less allocated the ride, but I knew she was a capable rider. Itworked out and she rode him several times again, winning at Musselburgh.

“She was over the moon that day at Fairyhouse, and her family. I met her mother at Bangor one day and she’s lovely too. I send my condolences to her family, my heart goes out to them.”

.



  1. Man dies after freak accident at Portarlington Golf Club
  2. AFL brain disease cases ‘tip of the iceberg’: US expert
  3. Bill for veterans' mental health care reaches $241m with 20,000 in rehab
  4. Jail for father who continued gaming after fatally injuring baby son
  5. VA unlawfully turned away vulnerable veterans for decades, study says, with 400,000 more at risk
  6. Brain Injuries Are Common in Battle. The Military Has No Reliable Test for Them.
  7. Fifty US troops left with brain injuries after Iranian rocket attack
  8. This Helmet Will Save Football. Actually, Probably Not.
  9. British man found guilty of Australian Amy Parsons' murder in London
  10. My once-vibrant husband died of ALS, and my complicated grief is deep
  11. Program to Prevent Suicide by Veterans Earns Bipartisan Support
  12. Sporty teens with concussions are three times more likely to be depressed
  13. Just one season of playing football—even without a concussion—can cause brain damage
  14. Startups fighting a 'bulletproof' mentality in men's health
  15. 'His personality changed': Michael Hutchence's sister on his traumatic brain injury
  16. Toddler suffers 'catastrophic brain injury' in alleged beating
  17. Cyclist, 70, left with head and spinal injuries after being hit by car
  18. 'Choked to the point of brain damage': Ice scourge fuels domestic violence
  19. Mass Murderer Possible undiagnosed brain damage
  20. Savage attack in Melbourne's north leaves tourist with bleeding to the brain, broken jaw
  21. Link between concussion and brain damage to ensure AFL debate rages
  22. Sports commentator Billy J Smith dies after a fall
  23. Surgeon killer could be first to get10-year term under one-punch laws
  24. Liam Neeson's nephew Ronan Sexton dies, years after serious fall
  25. Toddler burnt with lighter and hit every day in lead-up to her death, court told
  26. Patron filmed unconscious, held around neck as guard evicts him from hotel
  27. FA Cup set to introduce concussion substitute trial this season
  28. Teen fighting for life after Healesville car park brawl
  29. Police discover critically injured man at Logan Village address
  30. 'Don't ask me for compassion': Angry Anderson has not forgiven his son's killer
  31. Brain Injuries Remain Undiagnosed in Thousands of Soldiers
  32. Man dies in hospital after falling to punch in Fortitude Valley
  33. Maradona to be discharged within days, says doctor
  34. Cricket bat bashing victim fights for life after Ballajura pub brawl
  35. Diego Maradona, World Cup-winning football superstar, set to undergo brain surgery
  36. 'Country footy is way behind': The missing concussion discussion in local level Aussie Rules
  37. Autistic girls going undiagnosed due to ‘camouflaging’ behaviour, study says
  38. Lisa Montgomery to be first female federal inmate executed in 67 years
  39. Man dies after being shoved to the ground in New York mask altercatio
  40. Thomas had a rare brain cancer and no good options. Then he joined a clinical trial
  41. Nearly One-Third of Covid Patients in Study Had Altered Mental State
  42. Shaun Smith supportive of daughter Amy, signed by AFLW club North Melbourne
  43. Texas residents warned of tap water tainted with brain-eating microbe
  44. 'It's been a big day for me': Smith wants change after $1.4m concussion payout
  45. Damage Assessment
  46. What are CTE and concussion and how do they affect athletes?
  47. Danny Frawley was suffering from chronic brain disease when he died
  48. Elon Musk unveils brain computer implanted in pigs
  49. Portland truck driver apparently kicked unconscious as unrest continues
  50. Treatment for aggressive brain cancer shows promise in early trial
  51. Four-year-old injured after motorbike crashes through barriers at Sydney race
  52. 'Dangerous behaviour': Horror crash in sprint to finish leaves rider fighting for life
  53. Father charged with murder over death of six-month-old baby Beau
  54. Sickening Michael Chee Kam concussion overshadows gritty Eels win
  55. We asked veterans to respond to The Post’s reporting on Clint Lorance and his platoon. Here’s what they said.
  56. Doctors find brain issues linked to Covid-19 patients – study
  57. Widow of heart surgeon killed in one-punch attack sues Melbourne hospital
  58. Crowdfunding raises £30,000 for veteran's brain tumour surgery
  59. Boy in critical condition after fall at Sydney primary school
  60. 'I began to wonder if I would be better off ending my life': The invisible war wounds
  61. VA unlawfully turned away vulnerable veterans for decades, study says, with 400,000 more at risk
  62. Brain wiring could be behind learning difficulties, say experts
  63. Concussion: there's no knockout answer
  64. CTE discovered in Polly Farmer's brain in AFL-first
  65. Six-week-old baby nearly killed in ice-fuelled attack, court told
  66. Former hard man Ron Gibbs' chilling admission as head knocks take toll
  67. An Olympic Hockey Hero, a Violent Crime and the Specter of Brain Trauma
  68. Traumatic brain injury is a signature wound of the wars in Iraq and Afghanistan. But the military still has no objective way of diagnosing it in the field.
  69. More than 100 US troops suffered traumatic brain suffered traumatic brain in Iran strike,to report
  70. Man, 28, fighting for life nearly two weeks after Southbank attack
  71. NRL pledges initial $250,000 for landmark concussion study
  72. Veterans criticize Trump's downplaying of US troops' brain injuries
  73. Trump should apologize for minimizing troops’ injuries, VFW says.
  74. Fifty US troops left with brain injuries after Iranian rocket attack
  75. Can heading a football lead to dementia? The evidence is growing
  76. Mobile phones cause tumours, Italian court rules, in defiance of evidence
  77. Scientists create decoder to turn brain activity into speech
  78. Woman reportedly wakes up from coma after 27 years
  79. Enraged Qld dad who killed toddler jailed
  80. 'We thought it would be wonderful - we didn't know what was to come'
  81. 'We thought it would be wonderful - we didn't know what was to come'
  82. Graham must wake up to dangers of concussion
  83. Footballers focus on concussion, but there are many other risk factors
  84. Ex-AFL player sues club after retiring because of concussion
  85. When will we stop butting heads over sporting concussion?
  86. Why people with brain implants are afraid of automatic doors
  87. Christchurch mosque shooting victim, 4, suffering brain damage
  88. Link between concussion and brain damage to ensure AFL debate rages

Page 5 of 35

Latest News

  • ‘Extremely dark’: Safety concerns raised before jockey’s death on ‘bush track’ 
    ‘Extremely dark’: Safety concerns raised before jockey’s death on ‘bush track’ The Age Adam Cooper November 22, 2021 As the two racehorses cantered that...
  • ‘Extremely dark’: Safety concerns raised before jockey’s death on ‘bush track’ 
     Extremely dark’: Safety concerns raised before jockey’s death on ‘bush track’  The Age By Adam Cooper  November 22, 2021  As the two racehorses...
  • The Unclaimed soldier
    The Unclaimed soldier Thousands of veterans, especially from the Vietnam era, die alone every year The Washington Post Democracy Dies in Darkness By...
  • London cabbies’ brains are being studied for their navigating skills. It could help Alzheimer’s research.
    London cabbies’ brains are being studied for their navigating skills. It could help Alzheimer’s research. The Washington Post By Cathy Free 2/11/2021 London...
  • Former Tiger seeks to lift damages bid with concussion claim
    Former Tiger seeks to lift damages bid with concussion claim The Age By Jon Pierik October 22, 2021 A former Richmond footballer is pushing to...
  • Mother jailed in UK over baby’s injuries blames former partner at appeal
    Mother jailed in UK over baby’s injuries blames former partner at appeal  The Guardian UK Hannah Summers Thu 28 Oct 2021  A woman has told the appeal...